[LINK] O/T Toxoplasma gondii
stephen at melbpc.org.au
stephen at melbpc.org.au
Fri Dec 28 14:17:04 AEDT 2012
How Your Cat Is Making You Crazy
BY Kathleen McAuliffe, Atlantic Magazine
http://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2012/03/how-your-cat-is-
making-you-crazy/308873/ (and)
http://www.heraldsun.com.au/news/the-other-side/cat-poo-contains-a-common-
bug-that-may-be-controlling-your-mind/story-e6frfhk6-1226532097725
No one would accuse Jaroslav Flegr of being a conformist. A self-
described sloppy dresser, the 53-year-old Czech scientist has the
contemplative air of someone habitually lost in thought, and his still-
youthful, square-jawed face is framed by frizzy red hair that encircles
his head like a ring of fire.
Certainly Flegrs thinking is jarringly unconventional. Starting in the
early 1990s, he began to suspect that a single-celled parasite in the
protozoan family was subtly manipulating his personality, causing him to
behave in strange, often self-destructive ways. And if it was messing
with his mind, he reasoned, it was probably doing the same to others.
The parasite, which is excreted by cats in their feces, is called
Toxoplasma gondii (T. gondii or Toxo for short) and is the microbe that
causes toxoplasmosisthe reason pregnant women are told to avoid cats
litter boxes. Since the 1920s, doctors have recognized that a woman who
becomes infected during pregnancy can transmit the disease to the fetus,
in some cases resulting in severe brain damage or death. T. gondii is
also a major threat to people with weakened immunity: in the early days
of the AIDS epidemic, before good antiretroviral drugs were developed, it
was to blame for the dementia that afflicted many patients at the
diseases end stage. Healthy children and adults, however, usually
experience nothing worse than brief flu-like symptoms before quickly
fighting off the protozoan, which thereafter lies dormant inside brain
cellsor at least thats the standard medical wisdom.
But if Flegr is right, the latent parasite may be quietly tweaking the
connections between our neurons, changing our response to frightening
situations, our trust in others, how outgoing we are, and even our
preference for certain scents. And thats not all. He also believes that
the organism contributes to car crashes, suicides, and mental disorders
such as schizophrenia. When you add up all the different ways it can harm
us, says Flegr, Toxoplasma might even kill as many people as malaria, or
at least a million people a year.
An evolutionary biologist at Charles University in Prague, Flegr has
pursued this theory for decades in relative obscurity. Because he
struggles with English and is not much of a conversationalist even in his
native tongue, he rarely travels to scientific conferences. That may be
one of the reasons my theory is not better known, he says. And, he
believes, his views may invite deep-seated opposition. There is strong
psychological resistance to the possibility that human behavior can be
influenced by some stupid parasite, he says. Nobody likes to feel like
a puppet. Reviewers [of my scientific papers] may have been offended.
Another more obvious reason for resistance, of course, is that Flegrs
notions sound an awful lot like fringe science, right up there with UFO
sightings and claims of dolphins telepathically communicating with
humans.
But after years of being ignored or discounted, Flegr is starting to gain
respectability. Psychedelic as his claims may sound, many researchers,
including such big names in neuroscience as Stanfords Robert Sapolsky,
think he could well be onto something. Flegrs studies are well
conducted, and I can see no reason to doubt them, Sapolsky tells me.
Indeed, recent findings from Sapolskys lab and British groups suggest
that the parasite is capable of extraordinary shenanigans. T. gondii,
reports Sapolsky, can turn a rats strong innate aversion to cats into an
attraction, luring it into the jaws of its No. 1 predator. Even more
amazing is how it does this: the organism rewires circuits in parts of
the brain that deal with such primal emotions as fear, anxiety, and
sexual arousal. Overall, says Sapolsky, this is wild, bizarre
neurobiology. Another academic heavyweight who takes Flegr seriously is
the schizophrenia expert E. Fuller Torrey, director of the Stanley
Medical Research Institute, in Maryland. I admire Jaroslav for doing
[this research], he says. Its obviously not politically correct, in
the sense that not many labs are doing it. Hes done it mostly on his
own, with very little support. I think it bears looking at. I find it
completely credible.
Whats more, many experts think T. gondii may be far from the only
microscopic puppeteer capable of pulling our strings. My guess is that
there are scads more examples of this going on in mammals, with parasites
weve never even heard of, says Sapolsky.
Familiar to most of us, of course, is the rabies virus. On the verge of
killing a dog, bat, or other warm-blooded host, it stirs the animal into
a rage while simultaneously migrating from the nervous system to the
creatures saliva, ensuring that when the host bites, the virus will live
on in a new carrier. But aside from rabies, stories of parasites
commandeering the behavior of large-brained mammals are rare. The far
more common victims of parasitic mind controlat least the ones we know
aboutare fish, crustaceans, and legions of insects, according to Janice
Moore, a behavioral biologist at Colorado State University. Flies, ants,
caterpillars, wasps, you name itthere are truckloads of them behaving
weirdly as a result of parasites, she says.
Consider Polysphincta gutfreundi, a parasitic wasp that grabs hold of an
orb spider and attaches a tiny egg to its belly. A wormlike larva emerges
from the egg, and then releases chemicals that prompt the spider to
abandon weaving its familiar spiral web and instead spin its silk thread
into a special pattern that will hold the cocoon in which the larva
matures. The possessed spider even crochets a specific geometric design
in the net, camouflaging the cocoon from the wasps predators.
Flegr himself traces his lifes work to another master of mind control.
Almost 30 years ago, as he was reading a book by the British evolutionary
biologist Richard Dawkins, Flegr was captivated by a passage describing
how a flatworm turns an ant into its slave by invading the ants nervous
system. A drop in temperature normally causes ants to head underground,
but the infected insect instead climbs to the top of a blade of grass and
clamps down on it, becoming easy prey for a grazing sheep. Its mandibles
actually become locked in that position, so theres nothing the ant can
do except hang there in the air, says Flegr. The sheep grazes on the
grass and eats the ant; the worm gains entrance into the ungulates gut,
which is exactly where it needs to be in order to completeas the Lion
King song goesthe circle of life. It was the first I learned about this
kind of manipulation, so it made a big impression on me, Flegr says.
After he read the book, Flegr began to make a connection that, he readily
admits, others might find crazy: his behavior, he noticed, shared
similarities with that of the reckless ant. For example, he says, he
thought nothing of crossing the street in the middle of dense
traffic, and if cars honked at me, I didnt jump out of the way. He
also made no effort to hide his scorn for the Communists who ruled
Czechoslovakia for most of his early adulthood. It was very risky to
openly speak your mind at that time, he says. I was lucky I wasnt
imprisoned. And during a research stint in eastern Turkey, when the
strife-torn region frequently erupted in gunfire, he recalls being very
calm. In contrast, he says, my colleagues were terrified. I wondered
what was wrong with myself.
His bewilderment continued until 1990, when he joined the biology faculty
of Charles University. As it happened, the 650-year-old institution had
long been a world leader in documenting the health effects of T. gondii,
as well as developing methods for detecting the parasite. In fact, just
as Flegr was arriving, his colleagues were searching for infected
individuals on whom to test their improved diagnostic kits, which is how
he came to be asked one day to roll up his sleeve and donate blood. He
discovered that he had the parasiteand just possibly, he thought, the
key to his baffling self-destructive streak.
He delved into T. gondiis life cycle. After an infected cat defecates,
Flegr learned, the parasite is typically picked up from the soil by
scavenging or grazing animalsnotably rodents, pigs, and cattleall of
which then harbor it in their brain and other body tissues. Humans, on
the other hand, are exposed not only by coming into contact with litter
boxes, but also, he found, by drinking water contaminated with cat feces,
eating unwashed vegetables, or, especially in Europe, by consuming raw or
undercooked meat. Hence the French, according to Flegr, with their love
of steak prepared saignantliterally, bleedingcan have infection rates
as high as 55 percent. (Americans will be happy to hear that the parasite
resides in far fewer of them, though a still substantial portion: 10 to
20 percent.) Once inside an animal or human host, the parasite then needs
to get back into the cat, the only place where it can sexually reproduce
and this is when, Flegr believed, behavioral manipulation might come into
play.
Researchers had already observed a few peculiarities about rodents with
T. gondii that bolstered Flegrs theory. The infected rodents were much
more active in running wheels than uninfected rodents were, suggesting
that they would be more-attractive targets for cats, which are drawn to
fast-moving objects. They also were less wary of predators in exposed
spaces. Little, however, was known about how the latent infection might
influence humans, because we and other large mammals were widely presumed
to be accidental hosts, or, as scientists are fond of putting it, a dead
end for the parasite. But even if we were never part of the parasites
life cycle, Flegr reasoned, mammals from mouse to man share the vast
majority of their genes, so we might, in a case of mistaken identity,
still be vulnerable to manipulations by the parasite.
In the Soviet-stunted economy, animal studies were way beyond Flegrs
research budget. But fortunately for him, 30 to 40 percent of Czechs had
the latent form of the disease, so plenty of students were available to
serve as very cheap experimental animals. He began by giving them and
their parasite-free peers standardized personality testsan inexpensive,
if somewhat crude, method of measuring differences between the groups. In
addition, he used a computer-based test to assess the reaction times of
participants, who were instructed to press a button as soon as a white
square popped up anywhere against the dark background of the monitor.
The subjects who tested positive for the parasite had significantly
delayed reaction times. Flegr was especially surprised to learn, though,
that the protozoan appeared to cause many sex-specific changes in
personality. Compared with uninfected men, males who had the parasite
were more introverted, suspicious, oblivious to other peoples opinions
of them, and inclined to disregard rules. Infected women, on the other
hand, presented in exactly the opposite way: they were more outgoing,
trusting, image-conscious, and rule-abiding than uninfected women.
The findings were so bizarre that Flegr initially assumed his data must
be flawed. So he tested other groupscivilian and military populations.
Again, the same results. Then, in search of more corroborating evidence,
he brought subjects in for further observation and a battery of tests, in
which they were rated by someone ignorant of their infection status. To
assess whether participants valued the opinions of others, the rater
judged how well dressed they appeared to be. As a measure of
gregariousness, participants were asked about the number of friends
theyd interacted with over the past two weeks. To test whether they were
prone to being suspicious, they were asked, among other things, to drink
an unidentified liquid.
The results meshed well with the questionnaire findings. Compared with
uninfected people of the same sex, infected men were more likely to wear
rumpled old clothes; infected women tended to be more meticulously
attired, many showing up for the study in expensive, designer-brand
clothing. Infected men tended to have fewer friends, while infected women
tended to have more. And when it came to downing the mystery fluid,
reports Flegr, the infected males were much more hesitant than
uninfected men. They wanted to know why they had to do it. Would it harm
them? In contrast, the infected women were the most trusting of all
subjects. They just did what they were told, he says.
Why men and women reacted so differently to the parasite still mystified
him. After consulting the psychological literature, he started to suspect
that heightened anxiety might be the common denominator underlying their
responses. When under emotional strain, he read, women seek solace
through social bonding and nurturing. In the lingo of psychologists,
theyre inclined to tend and befriend. Anxious men, on the other hand,
typically respond by withdrawing and becoming hostile or antisocial.
Perhaps he was looking at flip sides of the same coin.
Closer inspection of Flegrs reaction-time results revealed that infected
subjects became less attentive and slowed down a minute or so into the
test. This suggested to him that Toxoplasma might have an adverse impact
on driving, where constant vigilance and fast reflexes are critical. He
launched two major epidemiological studies in the Czech Republic, one of
men and women in the general population and another of mostly male
drivers in the military. Those who tested positive for the parasite, both
studies showed, were about two and a half times as likely to be in a
traffic accident as their uninfected peers.
When I met Flegr for the first time, last September, at his office on the
third floor of Charles Universitys Biological Sciences building, I was
expecting something of a wild man. But once you get past the riotous red
hair, his style is understated. Thin and slight of build, hes soft-
spoken, precise with his facts, andtrue to his Toxo statusclad in old
sneakers, faded bell-bottom jeans, and a loose-fitting button-up shirt.
As our conversation proceeds, I discover that his latest findings have
becometo quote Alice in Wonderlandcuriouser and curiouser, which may
explain why his forehead has the deep ruts of a chronic worrier, or
someone perpetually perplexed.
Hes published some data, he tells me, that suggest infected males might
have elevated testosterone levels. Possibly for that reason, women shown
photos of these men rate them as more masculine than pictures of
uninfected men. I want to investigate this more closely to see if its
true, he says. Also, it could be women find infected men more
attractive. Thats something else we hope to test.
Meanwhile, two Turkish studies have replicated his studies linking
Toxoplasma to traffic accidents. With up to one-third of the world
infected with the parasite, Flegr now calculates that T. gondii is a
likely factor in several hundred thousand road deaths each year. In
addition, reanalysis of his personality-questionnaire data revealed that,
just like him, many other people who have the latent infection feel
intrepid in dangerous situations. Maybe, he says, thats another
reason they get into traffic accidents. They dont have a normal fear
response.
Its almost impossible to hear about Flegrs research without wondering
whether youre infectedespecially if, like me, youre a cat owner, favor
very rare meat, and identify even a little bit with your Toxo sex
stereotype. So before coming to Prague, Id gotten tested for the
parasite, but I didnt yet know the results. It seemed a good time to see
what his intuition would tell me. Can you guess from observing someone
whether they have the parasitemyself, for example?, I ask.
No, he says, the parasites effects on personality are very subtle.
If, as a woman, you were introverted before being infected, he says, the
parasite wont turn you into a raving extrovert. It might just make you a
little less introverted. Im very typical of Toxoplasma males, he
continues. But I dont know whether my personality traits have anything
to do with the infection. Its impossible to say for any one individual.
You usually need about 50 people who are infected and 50 who are not, in
order to see a statistically significant difference. The vast majority of
people will have no idea theyre infected.
Still, he concedes, the parasite could be very bad news for a small
percentage of peopleand not just those who might be at greater risk for
car accidents. Many schizophrenia patients show shrinkage in parts of
their cerebral cortex, and Flegr thinks the protozoan may be to blame for
that. He hands me a recently published paper on the topic that he co-
authored with colleagues at Charles University, including a psychiatrist
named Jiri Horacek. Twelve of 44 schizophrenia patients who underwent MRI
scans, the team found, had reduced gray matter in the brainand the
decrease occurred almost exclusively in those who tested positive for T.
gondii. After reading the abstract, I must look stunned, because Flegr
smiles and says, Jiri had the same response. I dont think he believed
it could be true. When I later speak with Horacek, he admits to having
been skeptical about Flegrs theory at the outset. When they merged the
MRI results with the infection data, however, he went from being a
doubter to being a believer. I was amazed at how pronounced the effect
was, he says. To me that suggests the parasite may trigger
schizophrenia in genetically susceptible people.
One might be tempted to dismiss the bulk of Flegrs work as hokumthe
fanciful imaginings of a lone, eccentric scholarwere it not for the
pioneering research of Joanne Webster, a parasitologist at Imperial
College London. Just as Flegr was embarking on his human trials, Webster,
then a freshly minted Ph.D., was launching studies of Toxo-infected
rodents, reasoning, just as Flegr did, that as hosts of the parasite,
they would be likely targets for behavioral manipulation.
She quickly confirmed, as previous researchers had shown, that infected
rats were more active and less cautious in areas where predators lurk.
But then, in a simple, elegant experiment, she and her colleagues
demonstrated that the parasite did something much more remarkable. They
treated one corner of each rats enclosure with the animals own odor, a
second with water, a third with cat urine, and the last corner with the
urine of a rabbit, a creature that does not prey on rodents. We thought
the parasite might reduce the rats aversion to cat odor, she told
me. Not only did it do that, but it actually increased their attraction.
They spent more time in the cat-treated areas. She and other scientists
repeated the experiment with the urine of dogs and minks, which also prey
on rodents. The effect was so specific to cat urine, she says, that we
call it fatal feline attraction.
She began tagging the parasite with fluorescent markers and tracking its
progress in the rats bodies. Given the surgically precise way the
microbe alters behavior, Webster anticipated that it would end up in
localized regions of the brain. But the results defied expectations. We
were quite surprised to find the cyststhe parasites dormant formall
over the brain in what otherwise appeared to be a happy, healthy rat,
she says. Nonetheless, the cysts were most abundant in a part of the
brain that deals with pleasure (in human terms, were talking sex, drugs,
and rock and roll) and in another area thats involved in fear and
anxiety (post-traumatic stress disorder affects this region of the
brain). Perhaps, she thought, T. gondii uses a scattershot approach,
disseminating cysts far and wide, enabling a few of them to zero in on
the right targets.
To gain more clarity on the matter, she sought the aid of the
parasitologist Glenn McConkey, whose team at the University of Leeds was
probing the protozoans genome for signs of what it might be doing. The
approach brought to light a striking talent of the parasite: it has two
genes that allow it to crank up production of the neurotransmitter
dopamine in the host brain. We never cease to be amazed by the
sophistication of these parasites, Webster says.
Their findings, reported last summer, created immediate buzz. Dopamine is
a critical signaling molecule involved in fear, pleasure, and attention.
Furthermore, the neurotransmitter is known to be jacked up in people with
schizophreniaanother one of those strange observations about the
disease, like its tendency to erode gray matter, that have long puzzled
medical researchers. Antipsychotic medicine designed to quell
schizophrenic delusions apparently blocks the action of dopamine, which
had suggested to Webster that what it might really be doing is thwarting
the parasite. Scientists had already shown that adding the medicine to a
petri dish where T. gondii is happily dividing will stunt the organisms
growth. So Webster decided to feed the antipsychotic drug to newly
infected rats to see how they reacted. Lo and behold, they didnt develop
fatal feline attraction. Suddenly, attributing behavioral changes to the
microbe seemed much more plausible.
As the scientific community digested the British teams dopamine
discoveries, Robert Sapolskys lab at Stanford announced still more
attention-grabbing news. The neuroscientist and his colleagues found that
T. gondii disconnects fear circuits in the brain, which might help to
explain why infected rats lose their aversion to cat odor. Just as
startling, reports Sapolsky, the parasite simultaneously is able to
hijack some of the circuitry related to sexual arousal in the male rat
probably, he theorizes, by boosting dopamine levels in the reward-
processing part of the brain. So when the animal catches a whiff of cat
scent, the fear center fails to fully light up, as it would in a normal
rat, and instead the area governing sexual pleasure begins to glow. In
other words, he says, Toxo makes cat odor smell sexy to male rats.
The neurobiologist Ajai Vyas, after working with Sapolsky on this study
as a postdoctoral student, decided to inspect infected rats testicles
for signs of cysts. Sure enough, he found them thereas well as in the
animals semen. And when the rat copulates, Vyas discovered, the
protozoan moves into the females womb, typically infecting 60 percent of
her pups, before traveling on up to her own braincreating still more
vehicles for ferrying the parasite back into the belly of a cat.
Could T. gondii be a sexually transmitted disease in humans too? Thats
what we hope to find out, says Vyas, who now works at Nanyang
Technological University, in Singapore. The researchers also discovered
that infected male rats suddenly become much more attractive to
females. Its a very strong effect, says Vyas. Seventy-five percent of
the females would rather spend time with the infected male.
After I return from Prague, Flegr informs me that hes just had a paper
accepted for publication that, he claims, proves fatal feline attraction
in humans. By that he means that infected men like the smell of cat pee
or at least they rank its scent much more favorably than uninfected men
do. Displaying the characteristic sex differences that define many Toxo
traits, infected women have the reverse response, ranking the scent even
more offensive than do women free of the parasite. The sniff test was
done blind and also included urine collected from a dog, horse, hyena,
and tiger. Infection did not affect how subjects rated these other
samples.
Is it possible cat urine may be an aphrodisiac for infected men?, I
ask. Yes. Its possible. Why not? says Flegr. I think hes smiling at
the other end of the phone line, but Im not sure, which leaves me
wondering whether Ive stumbled onto a topic ripe for a Saturday Night
Live skit, or a matter worthy of medical concern. When I ask Sapolsky
about Flegrs most recent research, he says the effects Flegr is
reporting are incredibly cool. However, Im not too worried, in that the
effects on humans are not gigantic. If you want to reduce serious car
accidents, and you had to choose between curing people of Toxo infections
versus getting people not to drive drunk or while texting, go for the
latter in terms of impact.
In fact, Sapolsky thinks that Toxos inventiveness might even offer us
some benefits. If we can figure out how the parasite makes animals less
fearful, he says, it might give us insights into how to devise treatments
for people plagued by social-anxiety disorder, phobias, PTSD, and the
like. But frankly, he adds, this mostly falls into the Get a load of
this, can you believe what nature has come up with? category.
Webster is more circumspect, if not downright troubled. I dont want to
cause any panic, she tells me. In the vast majority of people, there
will be no ill effects, and those who are affected will mostly
demonstrate subtle shifts of behavior. But in a small number of cases,
[Toxo infection] may be linked to schizophrenia and other disturbances
associated with altered dopamine levelsfor example, obsessive-compulsive
disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and mood disorders.
The rat may live two or three years, while humans can be infected for
many decades, which is why we may be seeing these severe side effects in
people. We should be cautious of dismissing such a prevalent parasite.
The psychiatrist E. Fuller Torrey agreesthough he came to this viewpoint
from a completely different angle than either Webster or Flegr. His
opinion stems from decades of research into the root causes of
schizophrenia. Textbooks today still make silly statements that
schizophrenia has always been around, its about the same incidence all
over the world, and its existed since time immemorial, he says. The
epidemiology literature contradicts that completely. In fact, he says,
schizophrenia did not rise in prevalence until the latter half of the
18th century, when for the first time people in Paris and London started
keeping cats as pets. The so-called cat craze began among poets and left-
wing avant-garde Greenwich Village types, says Torrey, but the trend
spread rapidlyand coinciding with that development, the incidence of
schizophrenia soared.
Since the 1950s, he notes, about 70 epidemiology studies have explored a
link between schizophrenia and T. gondii. When he and his colleague
Robert Yolken, a neurovirologist at Johns Hopkins University, surveyed a
subset of these papers that met rigorous scientific standards, their
conclusion complemented the Prague groups discovery that schizophrenic
patients with Toxo are missing gray matter in their brains. Torrey and
Yolken found that the mental illness is two to three times as common in
people who have the parasite as in controls from the same region.
Human-genome studies, both scientists believe, are also in keeping with
that findingand might explain why schizophrenia runs in families. The
most replicated result from that line of investigation, they say,
suggests that the genes most commonly associated with schizophrenia
relate to the immune system and how it reacts to infectious agents. So in
many cases where the disease appears to be hereditary, they theorize,
what may in fact be passed down is an aberrant or deficient immune
response to invaders like T. gondii.
Epstein-Barr virus, mumps, rubella, and other infectious agents, they
point out, have also been linked to schizophreniaand there are probably
more as yet unidentified triggers, including many that have nothing to do
with pathogens. But for now, they say, Toxo remains the strongest
environmental factor implicated in the disorder. If I had to guess,
says Torrey, Id say 75 percent of cases of schizophrenia are associated
with infectious agents, and Toxo would be involved in a significant
subset of those.
Just as worrisome, says Torrey, the parasite may also increase the risk
of suicide. In a 2011 study of 20 European countries, the national
suicide rate among women increased in direct proportion to the prevalence
of the latent Toxo infection in each nations female population.
According to Teodor Postolache, a psychiatrist and the director of the
Mood and Anxiety Program at the University of Maryland School of
Medicine, a flurry of other studies, several conducted by his own team,
offers further support of T. gondiis link to higher rates of suicidal
behavior. These include investigations of general populations as well as
groups made up of patients with bipolar disorder, severe depression, and
schizophrenia, and in places as diverse as Turkey, Germany, and the
Baltimore/Washington area. Exactly how the parasite may push vulnerable
people over the edge is yet to be determined. Postolache theorizes that
what disrupts mood and the ability to control violent impulses may not be
the organism per se, but rather neurochemical changes associated with the
bodys immune response to it. As far-fetched as these ideas may sound,
says Postolache, the American Foundation for Suicide Prevention was
willing to put money behind this research.
Given all the nasty science swirling around this parasite, is it time for
cat lovers to switch their allegiance to other animals?
Even Flegr would advise against that. Indoor cats pose no threat, he
says, because they dont carry the parasite. As for outdoor cats, they
shed the parasite for only three weeks of their life, typically when
theyre young and have just begun hunting. During that brief period,
Flegr simply recommends taking care to keep kitchen counters and tables
wiped clean. (He practices what he preaches: he and his wife have two
school-age children, and two outdoor cats that have free roam of their
home.) Much more important for preventing exposure, he says, is to scrub
vegetables thoroughly and avoid drinking water that has not been properly
purified, especially in the developing world, where infection rates can
reach 95 percent in some places. Also, he advises eating meat on the well-
done sideor, if thats not to your taste, freezing it before cooking, to
kill the cysts.
As concerns about the latent infection mount, however, experts have begun
thinking about more-aggressive steps to counter the parasites spread.
Inoculating cats or livestock against T. gondii might be one way to
interrupt its life cycle, offers Johns Hopkins Robert Yolken. Moving
beyond prevention to treatment is a taller order. Once the parasite
becomes deeply ensconced in brain cells, routing it out of the body is
virtually impossible: the thick-walled cysts are impregnable to
antibiotics. Because T. gondii and the malaria protozoan are related,
however, Yolken and other researchers are looking among antimalarial
agents for more-effective drugs to attack the cysts. But for now,
medicine has no therapy to offer people who want to rid themselves of the
latent infection; and until solid proof exists that Toxo is as dangerous
as some scientists now fear, pharmaceutical companies dont have much
incentive to develop anti-Toxo drugs.
Yolken hopes that will change. To explain where we are in Toxo research
today, he says, the analogy I always give is the ulcer bacteria. We
first needed to find ways of treating the organism and showing that the
disease went away when you did that. We will have to show that when we
very effectively treat Toxoplasma, some portion of psychiatric illness
goes away.
But T. gondii is just one of an untold number of infectious agents that
prey on us. And if the rest of the animal kingdom is anything to go by,
says Colorado State Universitys Janice Moore, plenty of them may be
capable of tinkering with our minds. For example, she and Chris Reiber, a
biomedical anthropologist at Binghamton University, in New York, strongly
suspected that the flu virus might boost our desire to socialize. Why?
Because it spreads through close physical contact, often before symptoms
emergemeaning that it must find a new host quickly. To explore this
hunch, Moore and Reiber tracked 36 subjects who received a flu vaccine,
reasoning that it contains many of the same chemical components as the
live virus and would thus cause the subjects immune systems to react as
if theyd encountered the real pathogen.
The difference in the subjects behavior before and after vaccination was
pronounced: the flu shot had the effect of nearly doubling the number of
people with whom the participants came in close contact during the brief
window when the live virus was maximally contagious. People who had very
limited or simple social lives were suddenly deciding that they needed to
go out to bars or parties, or invite a bunch of people over, says
Reiber. This happened with lots of our subjects. It wasnt just one or
two outliers.
Reiber has her eye trained on other human pathogens that she thinks may
well be playing similar games, if only science could prove it. For
example, she says, many people at the end stages of AIDS and syphilis
express an intense craving for sex. So, too, do individuals at the
beginning of a herpes outbreak. These may just be anecdotal accounts, she
concedes, but based on her own findings, she wouldnt be surprised if
these urges come from the pathogen making known its will to survive.
Weve found all kinds of excuses for why we do the things we do,
observes Moore. My genes made me do it. My parents are to blame. Im
afraid we may have reached the point where parasites may have to be added
to the laundry list of excuses.
She has a point. In fact, Ive been wondering whether T. gondii might in
some small way be contributing to my extreme extroversionwhy I cant
resist striking up conversations everywhere I go, even when Im short of
time or with strangers Ill never see again. Then it occurs to me that
cysts in my brain might be behind my seesaw moods or even my splurges on
expensive clothes. Maybe, I think with mounting conviction, the real me
would have displayed better self-control, had I not been forced to swim
upstream against the will of an insidious parasite. With my feline pal
Pixie on my lap (for the record, shes an outdoor cat), I call to get the
results of my Toxo test. Negative. I dont have the latent infection.
I call to tell Flegr the good news. Even though Im relieved, I know my
voice sounds flat. Its strange to admit, I say, but I think Im a
little disappointed. He laughs. People who have cats often feel that
way, because they think the parasite explains why they behave this way or
that, he says. But, I protest, you thought the same way. Then it
hits me. I may have dodged T. gondii, but given our knack for fooling
ourselvesplus all those parasites out there that may also be playing
tricks on our mindscan anyone really know whos running the show
--
Message sent using MelbPC WebMail Server
More information about the Link
mailing list